by Ronald L. Rutowski Intent on seeing where she lays her egg, I am diligently following a Little Yellow female as
she flies low to the ground along a railbed in
south-central Florida. Suddenly a fast-flying male
zips into view and dives toward the female. He
flies rapidly around her as she continues
deliberately along. After a few seconds, and with the male
still in pursuit, she begins a slow upward flight
that takes the pair ten to twenty feet off the ground.
I almost lose them against the bright summer sky
but then the male suddenly breaks off his chase
and drops straight toward the ground before turning
to dart away. The female descends a moment later and by a less precipitous route, but once near
the ground she continues her search for a plant on which to lay her eggs, which I learned was
a legume, the partridge pea (see articles about cassias).
Three may be a crowd but butterfly relationships
not infrequently become a menage a trois. Here a female
Falcate Orangetip (without the orange tips) is wooed
by two males. April 28, 1993. Assunpink WMA, Monmouth Co.,
New Jersey (photo by Jeffrey Glassberg) My scientific life has been spent describing
the interactions that occur when butterflies meet
and
trying to understand what is going on and why. In spite of the difficulties of studying
behavioral interactions of these fast-flying aerial animals --
even determining the sex of the participants can
be a problem -- I persist in following butterflies
with stopwatch and notepad. In this article I hope
to give you a sense of what I and others have discovered about the sexual encounters
between butterflies, which you might put to use the
next time you are butterflying.
Males are from Mars and females are from Venus? The activities of males are primarily those
of finding, courting and mating with females, but various species (and sometimes males of
one species, at different times and places) use
different means to find females. Some males are
patrollers, with sexually driven individuals flying
through their environment looking for females here
and there, while others are perchers, selecting a
suitable site to sit and wait for females to appear.
Classic examples of patrollers include the male
whites and sulphurs. In this group, no male
stays long in one place as he courses about searching
for females. Among the best known of the perchers in North America are Painted Ladies and
Red Admirals. In these species, males perch in
the afternoon and patiently wait for females to
appear on hilltops or along paths and walkways.
The wonderful wing-bar display of the Barred Sulphur. Hey, if you've got it, flaunt
it. Lake Placid, Florida (photo by Ronald L. Rutowski) Females are another story. Usually they
are found by a male very soon after emerging from
the chrysalis. Or, they themselves fly to the
areas where the males have set up territories. In
either case they usually welcome a male's advances.
But after this initial mating they typically
become unreceptive to male courtship attempts and
focus their time and energies on egg-laying.
However, after laying eggs for a few days they may
become receptive again; if they succeed in mating, they
will then no longer accept a partner for another
period before (perhaps) becoming receptive once more.
Prospecting for mates When an approaching male draws near its target, what happens next depends upon
the target's identity. First, the male may quickly
veer off and continue on his way, as happens when
the approached individual is a fellow male or a
member of some other species. But, particularly
when two males belong to the same species, they may
chase one another back and forth for a bit before
parting company to go their separate ways.
Courtship with unwilling females Another option is for the female to land
and assume a posture that makes it exceedingly
difficult for the male to copulate. The best known
of these maneuvers is the "mate-refusal posture"
of white and sulphur butterflies in which the
female spreads her wings and points her abdomen
skyward (see photo, below). Males cannot make genital contact with a female in this stance and
so they usually give up after a frustrating 10 or
15 seconds of trying. Yet another option is for
the perched female to persistently and rapidly
open and close her wings while the male is trying
to contact her abdomen tip with his own, an action that buffets her harasser and eventually sends
him on his way.
First photos (I think) of the courtship dance
of Arctic Skippers. Starting from the upper left, this sequence
shows the synchronous wing flapping that the male and female engage in
as they run along stems and leaves. Then the male curls his
abdomen around to the tip of the females abdomen
and they mate. June 28, 1996. McNair, Lake Co., Minnesota (photos by Jeffrey Glassberg) Courtship with willing females Sexual behavior can be a bit more exciting in those species
in which the female requires that males perform more
elaborate courtship rituals. One of the best known butterfly courtships is that of the
Queen, described by Lincoln Brower and his colleagues
in the 1960's. The striking feature of this
courtship is that while the female is still on the wing, the
male moves in front of her and begins flying up
and down facing away from the female but very
close to her head. While doing this he everts a pair
of brush-like, scent-disseminating structures on
his abdomen, called hairpencils, bringing them
close to or even in contact with the female's antennas.
Hairpencils are found in most milkweed butterflies, but we rarely see them because they
are everted only during or just prior to courtship,
and then only briefly. After the male uses his
hairpencils for a minute or so, a receptive female lands on
the ground or vegetation and permits the male to couple with her.
Here, I must mention a well-known relative
of the Queen, the Monarch which engages in a sort of mating frenzy in the late winter just before
and as their well-known winter aggregations in
central Mexico and the West Coast of the United
States are breaking up. In sharp contrast to the hairpencilling display of their close cousin,
male Monarchs are more likely to grab flying
females and carry them to the ground where they
attempt to mate with them. This illustrates the
rather striking differences that can evolve in the
courtship rituals of even closely-related species.
Another species with a visually impressive male courtship display is the Gulf Fritillary.
The eager male lands alongside the female and
begins clapping his wings together about 5 or 10 times
per second. Our slow motion films of this display show that the female swings her antennas back
so that the one on the side next to the male is
inserted between his wings and is therefore embraced
by them with each clap of the male wings. Because males have special fringed scales along
certain veins on the dorsal (upper) surface of the
forewing, this display probably delivers a chemical
signal to the female that encourages her to remain still
and permit the male to couple.
In my experience, the most bizarre display
is that of the Barred Yellow, which ranges from
the southern United States into Central America. After the female lands, the following male
lands alongside the female. While his other three
wings remain closed over his back, he extends his forewing on the side next to the female so that it is directly in front of her. He then waves the
wing up and down in front of her several times a
second in such a way that on each up-stroke the
female's antenna is rubbed against the trailing edge of
his forewing. If properly stimulated by this
display, the female keeps her wings closed and bends
her abdomen ventrally so that it protrudes from
under the hindwings, which makes it readily available
to the male for coupling.
Eager males but choosy females What useful information might females get from the male's displays? One possibility is
that they learn whether or not the male belongs to
their species. Most mating between individuals of different species are unproductive and such
matings are a waste of time for both sexes. Bob
Silberglied and Chip Taylor demonstrated that Orange
Sulphur females distinguish males of their own species from those of Clouded Sulphur by
using chemical signals produced by the male and by
the ultraviolet light reflected by special scales on
the male's upperside wing surface. Interestingly,
the bright orange color of the male was found to
play little role in their attractiveness to females.
In another example, Western White females preferentially mated with males having
naturally darker markings on the margin of the
forewing above. Diane Wiernasz found that she
could increase a male's chances of mating if she
experimentally darkened the appropriate marks with
a marking pen. She concluded that this preference helps females avoid mating with males of a
closely related butterfly, Checkered White, which
occurs in the same areas.
In addition, females may also get
information during courtship about the male's quality
relative to other males of the same species. His
behavior and signals could tell her about the quality of
his genes and the quality of nutrients that he can
pass to the female during mating, both of which
could affect the survival chances or number of
offspring the female may ultimately produce.
Whether female butterflies gain such benefits from
actively choosing among males within their species
has only recently been an active area of
investigation in laboratories around the world. The
experiments and results to date do not unfortunately
allow simple or broad conclusions and so are beyond
the scope of this brief overview. Suffice it to say
that, because most butterflies do not behave well
in cages, the study of mate choice and its
advantages under controlled conditions is proving to be
a difficult nut to crack.
Sometimes males choose inappropriate partners. Opposite page: A male
Sachem attempts to mate with a male Whirlabout while both are perched on
an embarrassed butterflier! Oct. 27, 1996. Roma, Starr Co., Texas (photo by Jeffrey Glassberg) As a final note on sexual interactions,
although the general pattern is that males approach
females, on occasion females will be seen actively
approaching and chasing males. These interactions have been interpreted as attempts by females
to draw the attention of males and are therefore called courtship solicitation. In my lab we
have studied this behavior in Checkered Whites and found that, not surprisingly, the females that
do this are most likely to be either virgins or
females who have not mated for some time. In both
cases females may be anxious to mate to acquire or renew sperm supplies and get on with the
business of producing eggs. My friend, Per-Olof
Wickman, at Stockholm University in Sweden found a
similar behavior in the Common Ringlet, a species
that occurs in both Sweden and North America.
Territorial males A male approaches a female who clearly is not interested, and who
sticks her abdomen into the air to prove it.
July 26, 1993. Chappaqua, Westchester Co., New York (photo by Jeffrey Glassberg) In Arizona, male Pipevine Swallowtails occupy and defend hilltop territories where they
wait for females. John Alcock, Mike Carey, and I
have observed lengthy aerial interactions between
persistent intruders and territorial males, which spiral
around one another while rapidly gaining altitude.
In appearance, these interactions are very much like the ascending flights between males
and unreceptive females. However, these male-male ascending flights are actually fights that
require endurance and maneuverability as the
butterflies climb three hundred feet or more into the sky.
After one or more of these flights the
intruder, usually but not always, ends up leaving the area.
Thus, when we see an ascending flight we cannot automatically conclude that it is a male
interacting with an unreceptive female.
In a recent and fascinating study with
Black Swallowtails, Bob Lederhouse and Mark
Scriber at Michigan State University demonstrated
that coloration may play a role in determining the outcome of these fights. They altered the color
of newly emerged males with marking pens to make them look like females. They then released
altered and unaltered males into an area where males
were defending mating territories and recorded the success of the released males in becoming
territory residents. The clear result was that males made
to look like females were less successful in
acquiring a territory, which adds a new twist to our
understanding of the role of coloration in
interactions between butterflies.
So from this quick review what general statements can be made about what happens
when butterflies meet? Clearly, the form of the
interactions we observe is determined by the
sexual identity of the interactants as well as their
mating histories, coloration, and a host of other
variables, some of which we do not yet fully understand.
As a result, we can often infer from the form of
the interactions what is going on and why, but not
with complete certainty. For events like
ascending flights, the participants and function vary
depending on the species and context. The relevant
caveat is to interpret what we see with care. In any
event, the interactions of butterflies can be complex
and beautiful, give us a bit of a window into how
they experience their world and mates, and perhaps cause us to reflect on the interactions we have
with them and members of our own species.
Adults are the sexually active stage of the
butterfly life cycle and so reproducing is the goal behind
all of their behavior. However, male and female butterflies are very different creatures, who
use different tactics to attain this goal.
Most butterfly interactions begin when a male, eager to court and mate, rushes toward any
other butterfly that even vaguely resembles a female.
Males are surprisingly indiscriminate [well,
maybe its not such a big surprise -- ed.]. Little
Yellow males will approach almost any yellow object
of the appropriate size, including bits of
surveyor's tape, cast off candy bar wrappers, and
dead leaves. Males of the Empress Leilia in
Arizona enthusiastically fly toward other kinds of
insects passing nearby, even birds on occasion.
Alternatively, a male may begin flying rapidly and persistently around the flying butterfly
for longer than a few seconds. This behavior is a
good indication the male has located a female but
what happens next depends on whether she is
receptive or not. If, as happens in most cases, she is not,
then she may signal to the pursuing male that he is
out of luck by beginning an ascending flight like
that described at the beginning of this article.
Invariably the male gets the message and gives up
his chase usually after 10 or 15 seconds of the
slow, upward flight.
The behavior of a sexually receptive female is much more congenial from the male's
perspective. A willing female slows her forward flight
and finds a place to land when a suitable partner comes close. With little obvious courtship, he will land
a little behind the female, scramble up beside
her, and begin probing the rear margin of her
hindwings with his abdomen. Once the tip of the
male's abdomen meets that of the female, the
genitalia link and copulation begins, all in a few seconds time.
Once coupled, the pair sits quietly for a period that depends on the
species but is typically 30 minutes to an hour.
Most butterfly courtships probably involve
chemical and visual signals, even in species
without elaborate or complex displays. Why do
males provide these signals and why do females
require them? Not all virgin females agree to mate,
even after vigorous courtship. Perhaps females are sizing up the male's attractiveness from his
behavior and the quality and quantity of signals he
is delivering.
Not all complex and lengthy interactions in
butterflies are between males and females. In
many species in which males use a sit-and-wait or perching tactic to locate females the males
defend the same perching location for day after day
and do not tolerate other males perching anywhere nearby. In other words, they are territorial.
Resident males approach intruders and chase them from the territory, but intruders may
resist, leading to intensely competitive interactions
between the resident and intruder.
30 Dec 1997 / Main Page